.

Friday, March 29, 2019

Cross Linguistic Influence English Language Essay

Cross Linguistic Influence incline Language EssayThe explore bea of this story is a cross- lingual influence or as other(a)wise known exaltation. Specific completelyy, I jut out to examine orthographic conveyancing and its potential drop lay out on face primaeval speakers orthoepy of segmentals in German as their moment phrase.The situation that writing system can buoy influence second lyric poem orthoepy is acknow directge by m twain foreign address teachers. Nevertheless, at that place is not exuberant present for such(prenominal) influence and to a greater extent than research is needed (Bas massti, 2008). earlier research on orthographic transportation system has mainly looked at its do on strikeing and spelling (Jarvis Pavlenko, 2008). Cook and Bassetti (2005) argue that makeup brass transfer is separate from quarrel transfer maintaining that it is not so lots aspects of the language that whitethorn be carried over as the attributes of a special(prenominal) writing outline (p. 29). Jarvis and Pavlenko (2008) concur with the in a higher place statement however they as well emphasize the importance of the connection betwixt orthographic transfer and language use, since it relates to the orthographic operationuate on phonology and ill-doing versa. According to Jarvis and Pavlenko, during the teaching of L1literacy skills, which uses the phonic alphabet, important sound-letter residues be acquired which later can be transferred to the skill and use of the second language. Before proceeding any further, the con inclineion of both(prenominal) key impairment that be extensively used in this saucys theme as well as their definitions as proposed by the researchers in the country of SLA is required. The terms be writing system and orthography.The graduation exercise term to specify is writing system. Coulmas (1999) defines writing system (WS) as a set of telescopic or tactile signs to re empower unit s of language in a authoritative route (p. 560). There are various types of writing systems depending on what type of linguistic units they represent (Bassetti, 2005). Consequently, there are writing systems whose linguistic units are consonants hence the name consonantal WSs (for pillow slip, Hebrew and Arabic). Morphemic WSs are stand for by morphemes (Chinese) and alphabetic WSs are represented by ph wizardmes ( side of meat, German, and Spanish). In addition, there are in like manner syllabic WSs whose linguistic units are syllables (Japanese). This paper leave itemally focus on alphabetic writing systems of English and German languages which use the aforementioned(prenominal) script the Roman alphabet.The term writing system relates to orthography which is the second term and is defined as a set of rules for using script in a particular language, such as symbol- phoneme correspondences, capitalization, punctuation, etc. (Coulmas, 2003, p. 35). For example, in Englis h orthography the letter s is read as /s/ and in German orthography as /z/ (Benware, 1986). olibanum, the self aforesaid(prenominal)(prenominal) script- the Roman alphabet- is used disaccordently in the English and German orthographies. Orthographies of the aforesaid(prenominal) type of alphabetic writing system and script are also varied in their regularity of sound-symbol correspondences (Cook Bassetti, 2005). For instance, in English, the letter a maps to different phonemes in words such as park, bank, and ball, whereas in German the same letter a has only one pronunciation /a/ as in German nouns Park, Ball, Bank (Goswami, Ziegler, Richardson, 2005). English orthography is in truth inconsistent in terms of its sound-letter correspondences thus it is considered to have deep orthography. Languages such as German, Italian and Spanish have relatively consistent letter-to-sound correspondences, so their orthographies are shallow or as otherwise called phonologically transparent (Cook Bassetti, 2005). Thus, it should be easier for English native speakers to acquire German sound-letter correspondence rules than for German native speakers to acquire English sound-letter correspondence rules. Nevertheless, there has been observed a capacious depend of segmental mispronunciations by English native speakers nurture German as a foreign language. Therefore, the question arises whether the companionship of L1 orthography has an effect on how English native speakers pronounce segmentals in German or are there any other factors such as kin(p) berth or the age of attainment that contribute to the orthographic transfer?.Literature reviewThe focus of this paper is to explore whether the familiarity of L1 (English) orthography, to wit its sound-letter correspondence rules, is transferred to L2 (German) and whether it has any positive degree or prejudicial effects on L2 pronunciation of segmentals in self-aggrandising language learners of different growth t akes. The paper also aims at investigating whether there is a significant disagreement of orthographic transfer produced by learners at different proficiency levels (beginning, intermediate and advanced). Finally, the knowledge go out examine whether a lexical factor such as related to status has all positive or negative effect on the acquisition and use of German as a foreign language.A number of studies have examined orthographic effects on either spelling or reading. Many studies also have looked at the effects of L1 orthographic experience on L2 word learning and decoding. However, there has been little research examining the effects of orthography on phonology and vice versa. According to Hayes-Harb, Nicol, and Barker (2010) no research has examined spelling conventions of the native and second languages where both make out the same script. Although there is whatever evidence of the kind between orthographic and phonological representations in learning crude wording , to a greater extent research is needed to shed light on this phenomenon (Hayes-Harb et al., 2010).As this test examines orthographic transfer from English to German, the notion of cross-linguistic similarity should be taken into consideration in particular given the fact that both languages share a significant number of cross-linguistic similarities in consanguine, lexical, phonological, and writing systems areas. Ringbom and Jarvis (2009) discuss the importance of cross-linguistic similarities in foreign language learning. Specifically, they maintain that learners desire on their earlier linguistic knowledge and look for similarities rather than differences between the L1 and the TL. Ringbom and Jarvis also state that second language learners refer to their previous linguistic knowledge broadly at the beginning stages of second language acquisition. some other important factor such as phonetic awareness and phonetic sensitivity should not be neglected. Piske (2008) maintai ns that children develop phonetic sensitivity to delivery contrasts specific to their L1 long before they begin read and write and at the same time their ability to perceive non-native speech contrasts (L2) weakens with age. So for adult language learners perceiving non-native speech contrast and sounds that are particular to the tail end language is difficult. To develop phonological awareness and sensitivity would require an extensive reading and frequent exposure to target language. Considering this fact, Piske states that adult language learners are influenced by writing systems of their L1 during L2 acquisition. Consequently this results in learners pronunciation errors which are linked to the trustingness on L1 grapheme-phoneme correspondence rules. Especially this refers to the cases when sound-letter correspondences are inconsistent between the L1 and L2.Odlin (1989) also provides ample evidence compiled from different studies that demonstrate the clear effect of first l anguage influence on L2 speech production and orthography. Odlin states that similar writing systems in L1 and L2 represent a much easier labour for learners acquiring a new language. The same however, cannot be said of pronunciation, as other variables may influence decoding of written symbols in L2. Flege, Freida, Walley, and Randaza (1998) examined whether lexical factors such as frequency, familiarity, cognate status and the age of acquisition have an effect on production. The authors provide an insight into what role the cognate status of words can play and how it effects speech production. More specifically, Flege et al. state that degree of perceive relatedness can vary depending on degree of cross-linguistic similarities the L1 and L2 share. Specifically, they evidence to the law of proximity of cognate pairs in L1-L2 in their crockeding and especially in sound. This paper will also examine whether cognate pairs influence L2 pronunciation of target sounds. Based on the se statements it is also likely that English-speaking learners of German will rely on their knowledge of English sound-letter correspondence rules when speaking or reading in the target language. This may result in either mispronunciation of sealed sound-letter correspondences that are inconsistent with English orthography or it may have facilitatory effects on L2 phonology acquisition.In terms of findings several studies specially stand out. Young-Scholten (2002)1 provides evidence of orthographic effects on the production of German consonants by English speakers. Another consume conducted by Young-Scholten, Akita, and Cross (1999)2 also shows evidence of the effects of written representations on the pronunciation of consonant clusters in learners of Polish as a second language. These findings led the researchers to conclude that there is a coincidenceship between orthographic representations and L2 phonology. separate studies investigating the effects of orthography on second language phonology demonstrate that second language learners pronunciation can be influenced by orthographic input (Bassetti, 2007).Bassetti (2007) examined the effects of pinyin (a romanized version of the Chinese language) on pronunciation in learners of Chinese as a foreign language. The author predicted that orthographic representation of pinyin would result in learners not pronouncing the main vowel in the rhymes whose transcription does not represent that main vowel as in rhymes /iu/ which maps to iou, /ui/ uei and /un/ un. The results of the study suggest strong influence of pinyin orthographic rules on the pronunciation of Chinese rhymes. Specifically, learners of Chinese as a foreign language would scratch the main vowel, which is not present orthographically however they would always pronounce the same main vowel in the rhymes /you/, /wen/, and /wei/ (Bassetti, 2006 Bassetti, 2007).One of the most novel studies conducted by Hayes-Harb et al. (2010) provides some evide nce of orthographic influence on the acquisition of the phonological forms of new words. In this study, Hayes-Harb et al. recreated conditions in which subjects experienced learning new vocabulary in a new language. The aim of the study was to check over whether the posture of the written form of the new words affected the learning of their phonological form. Thus, native speakers of English were divided into ternion roots. The first pigeonholing was provided with the written forms of new words that were inconsistent with English spelling. The second sort was presented with words that were matching the English sound-letter correspondences. The third pigeonholing had only an auditory input and no written forms of the new words were provided. The results demonstrated that L1 orthography interfered with students ability to learn new words especially when new vocabulary differed from English sound-letter correspondences. While Hayes-Harb et al. (2010) provide some evidence of or thographic effects in the process of learning new phonological forms of words, the authors admit that the system is a novel one and in the forthcoming should be controlled by learners spelling ability.Previous studies show clear effects of a relationship between orthography and phonology in second language acquisition. a lot of the research has analyzed orthographies that use different writing systems, and as Hayes-Harb et al. (2010) point out very little research has been done investigating the effects of orthographies on L2 pronunciation within the same writing system and script. Therefore, the routine of this paper is to investigate further the notion of orthographic transfer by analyzing the relationship between orthography and phonology of two languages that share the same script and the following research questions are addressed in this paperIs orthographic transfer statistically present in the sample?Do results vary and are they statistically significant between different proficiency levels?What is the nature of relationship between error form and students perceptions of cognate status?Method2.1 ParticipantsThe participants of this study were 28 undergraduate American English speakers from Ohio University. The students were enrolled in the 100, 200 and ccc levels of German as a foreign language. The levels differ by proficiency. The 100 level is offered to sires, the 200 level is an intermediate level, and the 300 level is taught to the advanced students respectively. Each proficiency level consists of a trinity quarter sequence meeting four hours per week with regular homework assignments. Each year sequence of German series focuses on maturation all four language skills (listening, speaking, reading, and writing).2.2 Materials and proceduresThe study consisted of two casts the screening strain and the data aggregation phase. During the screening phase, students enrolled in all three proficiency levels volunteered to fill out an online langu age background fall out. The purpose of this survey was to drive potential participants for the study by controlling several variables that could have had an effect on the results. It was set up in such a way that participants could be screened based on their responses to the first three questions. Thus, those who did not condition for the study were automatically eliminated without going over all of the survey questions. commencement of all, it helped eliminate the students with any reading or speech dis pronounces. It also helped blockade those students who were taking another foreign language class along with German. The survey also eliminated the students who had taken a foreign language other than German less than a year ago, which could have contributed to transfer from that language into their knowledge and use of German. In addition, entropy such as students motivation, number of old age they have studied German and the time spent in German speaking countries was smo oth through this survey. As a result, out of 63 students who had volunteered to fill out the survey, 28 were qualified and participated in the data collection phase. The selected participants include nine beginners, eight intermediate-level students, and eleven advanced-level students.The data collection phase consisted of three deputes administered in the following order an in formal reading loudly task, a formal reading loudly task, and a perception task. solely three tasks were performed by the students during individual single session meetings go bading cardinal minutes.Reading aloud is considered to be the best way to test the knowledge of sound-letter correspondences. It provides good control and allows for comparison of speech samples from different subjects. Reading aloud is also a technique that is widely used in a foreign language class thanks to which participants feel more prospering during data collection (Madsen, 1983). During the informal reading task, students w ere asked to read aloud a German text which was adapted in order to target certain German consonants and consonant clusters as detailed in Tables 1 and 2. The stock sets (see Table 1) include incongruent grapheme-phoneme correspondences students presumably would have had the most barrier producing.Table 1. Incongruent grapheme-phoneme correspondencesSounds live on in both languages, but are represented with different lettersGerman phoneme-graphemecorrespondencesEnglish phoneme-graphemecorrespondences1w / v e.g., Wind vntv /v eg. van vn2v / f e.g., Vogel foglf / f eg. caramel fn3s / z e.g., sinken zknz / z eg. zone zon4s/ when followed by e.g., Sport prts / when followed by eg. shine an5s / when followed by e.g., Student tudnts / when followed by eg. shed dSounds are different in both languages, but are represented with the same letters6t/ ts e.g., Tradition traditsiont/ e.g., tradition trdhttp//sp.dictionary.com/dictstatic/dictionary/graphics/luna/thinsp.pngn7z/ ts e.g., Zimme r tsmz / z e.g., zone zon8ch/ e.g., sportlich prtlch/ t e.g., rich rtAlso is included a set of congruent phoneme-grapheme correspondences that exist in both English and German languages (see Table 3).Table 3. Congruent grapheme-phoneme correspondencesGrapheme-phoneme correspondences existing in both languagesGerman examplesEnglish examples12345b maps to bn maps to nl maps to lt maps to tp maps to pBank bakNuss ns vote out lant differentiate takPark parkBank bkNut ntLand lnd Tag tg Park prk To test students pronunciation of the targeted German segmentals in relation to potential transfer effects, each grapheme-phoneme correspondence listed in Tables 1 and 2 was encountered at least twice in the informal reading task and once in the formal reading task. Students readings in both tasks were enter in a soundproof recording laboratory using auditory sensation recording software. During the first task, students had one minute to read the text taciturnly before being recorded.The formal reading task followed the informal one and required the participants to read words with the targeted sounds in isolation. The words in the formal task were a subset of those used in the informal task and some of them were cognates with English. Finally, during the perception task students had to categorize a list of both cognate and non-cognate German words in terms of their similarity to any of the English words. Students had to rate the words on a three-point scale (1=same 2= similar 3= dissimilar) relying on their own perceptions. Table 3 beneath births examples of words students rated in the perception task.Table 3 Categorization of cognate and non-cognate wordsSame meaningFormSameSimilar dissimilarGer. WindEng. WindGer. SturmEng. stormGer. VielEng. manyGer. StudentEng. StudentGer. milchEng. MilkGer. VogelEng. birdGer. WinterEng. WinterGer. TemperaturEng. TemperatureGer. ZeitEng. timeGer. ferventEng. WarmGer. WetterEng. WeatherGer. ZimmerEng. roomGer. SportEng. SportGer. Natu rlichEgn. of courseGer. traurigEng. sadGer. TraditionEng. TraditionGer. SportlichEng. sportyGer. InformationEng. InformationGer. SonneEng. cheerGer. StrudelEng. StrudelResults from these tasks revealed whether the reliance on first language orthography had a positive or negative effect on students pronunciation of segmentals in German. The mispronunciations in the recordings were quantified, categorized and recorded in a data file. Their performance was then codified and entered in the data file. Finally, student survey responses and identifying info collected through the questionnaire were matched with participants reading performance results and the perception task. Once the survey information and performance results were merged, the data was de-identified to ensure the confidentiality of the participants.Data depth psychology and resultsIn order to address the first research question of whether orthographic transfer is statistically present in the sample, the Wilcoxon Signed- Rank Test, the nonparametric alternative to the paired-sample t-test was used to examine whether the participants pronunciation errors were statistically more prevalent in cases of incongruent sound-letter correspondences between the L1 and L2 than in cases of congruent sound-letter correspondences. That is, we tested whether the participants dispersion of errors and successes is keyed to the specific ways in which the L1 and L2 are related in terms of sound-letter correspondences. The results of the test revealed that orthographic transfer is statistically present in the sample (p=.00). Students indeed produced more errors in cases of incongruent sound-letter correspondences and there were zero number of mistakes in cases with congruent sound-letter correspondences.In order to address our second research question concerning whether there is any difference in the mean pees between proficiency level groups, The Kruskal Wallis test was performed. This test was conducted on the resul ts of the reading tasks of the experiment to test whether the subjects from the three proficiency level groups differed significantly in producing German segmentals , f, ts, p, t, z, v. It was assumed that the distribution of errors would be more prevalent in the beginner level group as opposed to intermediate and advanced level groups. The results demonstrated a significant difference in mean scores between all three proficiency groups for both informal and formal reading tasks (p =.009). The results of the descriptive statistics are summarized below in Table 5.Table 5 miserly error scores for each proficiency levelBeginners group mean scoreIntermediate group mean scoreAdvanced group mean scoreGroup MeanInformal reading task22.5015.588.0014.83Formal reading task20.4213.855.4112.64Cumulative error mean43.3730.4313.4127.90 legitimate N981128Finally, in our last research question we examined the relationship between students performance results in both reading tasks and their percept ions regarding cognate status of the words. For this purpose a correlation abridgment was conducted to examine what kind of relationship there exist between the number of errors produced in the target sounds and the number of students rating the words as same or dissimilar. As a result, correlation coefficient between error score and a number of students rating the words as same revealed a weak negative relationship between these two variables, with correlation coefficient r = -.47. On the other hand, correlation analysis between error score and a number of students rating the words as dissimilar showed a moderate positive relationship between the variables with r = .57.DiscussionThe present study is exploratory in its nature yet the results show evidence of orthographic transfer in the sample. By that we mean that students do tend to rely on their L1 knowledge of sound-letter correspondence rules specifically in the cases of incongruent sound-letter correspondences between the L1 and L2. The results also indicate a significant difference in mean error scores at different proficiency levels. Thus, the beginner level students produced the highest number of errors in targeted segmentals than students from intermediate and advanced levels. These findings support the intellect Ringbom and Jarviss (2009) stated that the second language learners rely more on their previous linguistic knowledge at an early stages of second language acquisition. This especially applies to the languages that share a significant number of cross-linguistic features in cognate, lexical, phonological and writing systems, as in case of English and German.The results of this study are also in line with previous findings of Piske (2008), which suggested that adult learners are influenced by the writing systems of their L1. A considerable number of errors were observed in producing in seven out of eight German segmentals , f, ts, p, t, z, ts with the exception of v which maps to w in German. For example, the highest number of errors students produced was the German consonant g in word final exam which maps to palatal fricative when preceded by front vowels as in traurig and consonantal cluster ch in word final which also maps to palatal in words such as Milch, natrlich, sportlich. Students produced these two sounds as k. They also disjointed German v which maps to f with its English counterpart v. The same was observed with the other two German consonants s,z which map to z,ts respectively and were pronounced as English phonemes s, z. This can be explained by the fact that all German consonants mentioned above are represented by the same graphemes in English, which resulted in students mispronunciations. raise results were obtained from the words that contained labio-dental v which in German maps to w grapheme. None of the students had errors producing this sound scorn the fact that the same grapheme /w/ maps to an approximant w in English. This could be taken as following it appears easier for students to produce sounds that are far apart in terms of their place of articulation than for instance, the sounds that vary only in their voicing. Thus students in this study had difficulty to devoice fricative f in German because it is represented by grapheme v, which in English maps to express fricative v. Participants of this study also struggled producing consonantal clusters st, sp which in German language map to t, p. Although English has phoneme , it exists in different phonetic environment and is never followed by p,t phonemes in onset. Therefore students confused these two consonantal clusters with their English counterparts and produced them as sp for example English sport or st as in English student.Lastly, the perception task yielded somewhat ambiguous results. It appears that there tycoon be a relationship between the error score and a number of students rating the words as dissimilar. Nonetheless, this needs to be investigated in the future research using a larger sample as there was insufficient amount of data to strongly support the findings. For more details regarding error score and the students perceptions see Table 6 in Appendix A.ConclusionThe purpose of this study was to find out to what extent English-speaking learners of German rely on the English orthography and whether the presence of cognate words results in mispronunciation in the target language or vice versa helps learners to acquire L2 phonology. Given what was found (a) evidence that English orthography, videlicet its grapheme-phoneme rules interferes with learners pronunciation of German segmentals, (b) evidence that learners were influenced by L1 writing system mostly at the beginning stages of second language acquisition, and (c) some evidence of potential relationship between the errors produced and students perceptions of cognate status, we competency consider some operable implications for foreign language classroom as well as futu re research implications.As Hayes-Herb et al. (2010) suggested language teachers might find it beneficial presenting new vocabulary using initially auditory input only. This may particularly refer to the non-cognates words as students in this study made more errors in words that they rated as dissimilar with English. At the same time, it may be helpful for learners to receive written input of the words that contain German w in word initial, as students produced zero errors, which might suggest that in this particular case written input positively influenced learners pronunciation. Therefore it is important for language instructors to be aware of orthographic transfer and its positive and negative effects so that they can address this information accordingly in their classroom. On the other hand more research is needed to support these findings. For this purpose a more comprehensive study with a larger sample would help to expand the current analysis of data and make the findings gen eralizable for a larger population of students studying German as a foreign language. Teacher surveys might reveal whether language instructors focus on orthographic transfer or is it something new to them as well as to what extent they focus on pronunciation of those specific sounds that might be problematic due to the reliance on L1 sound-letter correspondence rules in their classroom. The results can provide valuable insights that can be use to the teaching of German as a foreign language.Furthermore, given the ambit of this study only eight German segmentals were investigated. Examining other segmentals whose orthographic representations exist only in German (for example, , , , ) or segmentals whose sounds exist only in German but are represented by the same graphemes (for example German r which maps to several phonemes , , r depending on the in word position) would clearly provide better results on the way English and German orthographies act with German as a foreign langua ge phonology.To conclude, while the this study attempted to address questions on how orthography of L1 and its sound-letter correspondence rules can be transferred to L2 phonology resulting in mispronunciation of certain segmentals still more research on this topic is needed.

No comments:

Post a Comment